In recent years, it has been increasingly recognized that food choices are not only central to health, but also to global sustainability. In particular, production and consumption of meat have been linked to dying prematurely, as well as proven to be a central source of CO2 emissions responsible for ~15% of the total global CO2 emission [1] [2]. Thus, changing people’s diets from a meat-based to a vegetarian, may prove to be a necessary step in the battle against what has become a global food–health crisis as well as climate change.

Although changing people’s diets may, at first, seem like a highly invasive and unfavorable task to embark, it doesn’t necessarily have to be. According to behavioural research, many food decisions can be influenced by even small social and contextual factors, and simply manipulating such factors may provide as cheap, effective and non-invasive interventions, also known as ‘nudges’ [3] [4] [5] [6].

A particularly robust and effective strategy for influencing people’s choices, such as food choices, is by changing the default setting [7] [8]. At its most simple, a default is an option from a choice set that enters into force by default, unless the person actively chooses an alternative option [9]. Not only is changing the default effective, but when used benignly, people will also advocate the intervention – even though it means changing some of their meals from meat-based to vegetarian. In fact, a recent series of experiments done by the iNudgeyou team just published in Journal of Public Health illustrate just so. Here is what we did:

At three conferences, a total of 330 participants registering electronically were randomized into two groups: Group 1 (N=159) received a standard lunch registration presenting a non-vegetarian buffet as the default, but allowing the active choice of a vegetarian option; Group 2 (N=171) received a registration presenting a vegetarian buffet as the default, allowing the active choice of a non-vegetarian option.

 

Results:

In Group 1, where the standard non-vegetarian menu was the default, 6% of the participants chose the vegetarian option. But in group 2, where we had switched the default setting to a vegetarian menu, 87% of the participants chose the vegetarian option (see Fig. 1).

Thus, by simply changing the default, we managed to consistently and significantly influence conference participants’ food choices to a healthier and more sustainable choice.

But how then, did the participants respond when telling them about the intervention?

At the last of the three conferences, participants were informed in an individual questionnaire about the experiment, its result and that they had been a part of it. They were then asked whether they approved of the nudge or not. Of the 78 participants who responded, 90% approved the nudge.

Thus, changing the lunch-default to a vegetarian option shows to be an effective, generic, easy to scale and well-accepted nudge to promote healthy and sustainable food choices at conferences.

OPEN ACCESS to the academic paper: Nudging healthy and sustainable food choices: three randomized controlled field experiments using a vegetarian lunch-default as a normative signal

Sources:

[1] Pan, A., Sun, Q., Bernstein, A. M., Schulze, M. B., Manson, J. E., Stampfer, M. J., … & Hu, F. B. (2012). Red meat consumption and mortality: results from 2 prospective cohort studies. Archives of internal medicine, 172(7), 555-563.

[2] Scarborough, P., Appleby, P. N., Mizdrak, A., Briggs, A. D., Travis, R. C., Bradbury, K. E., & Key, T. J. (2014). Dietary greenhouse gas emissions of meat-eaters, fish-eaters, vegetarians and vegans in the UK. Climatic change, 125(2), 179-192.

[3] Apostolidis, C., & McLeay, F. (2016). Should we stop meating like this? Reducing meat consumption through substitution. Food policy, 65, 74-89.

[4] Macdiarmid, J. I., Douglas, F., & Campbell, J. (2016). Eating like there’s no tomorrow: Public awareness of the environmental impact of food and reluctance to eat less meat as part of a sustainable diet. Appetite, 96, 487-493.

[5] Wansink, B. (2010). From mindless eating to mindlessly eating better. Physiology & behavior, 100(5), 454-463.

[6] Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Press, 2008

[7] Johnson, E. J., Bellman, S., & Lohse, G. L. (2002). Defaults, framing and privacy: Why opting in-opting out. Marketing Letters, 13(1), 5-15.

[8] Jachimowicz, J. M., Duncan, S., Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2019). When and why defaults influence decisions: A meta-analysis of default effects. Behavioural Public Policy, 3(2), 159-186.

[9] Hansen, P. G., Schilling, M., & Malthesen, M. S. Nudging healthy and sustainable food choices: three randomized controlled field experiments using a vegetarian lunch-default as a normative signal. Journal of Public Health.


 

Brochure for the Masterclass in Nudging

By downloading the course brochure, you give consent for us to send email communication to the provided email address. You can unsubscribe at any time.

 

Thank you for your interest! Soon you will receive the course brochure in your inbox.

Brochure for kurset Nudge din Kommunikation

Tak for din interesse. Vi sender dig en mail med brochuren.

Brochure for kurset Nudging og det gode arbejdsmiljø

Tak for din interesse. Vi sender dig en mail med brochuren.

Brochure forMasterclass i Nudging

Tak for din interesse. Vi sender dig en mail med brochuren.

Brochure forGrunduddannelsen

Ved download af kursusbrochuren giver du accept til, at vi må sende e-mailkommunikation til den angivne e-mailadresse. – Du kan til enhver tid afmelde igen.

Tak for din interesse. Vi sender dig en mail med brochuren.

Shares
X